- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Evaluation of apparently healthy Egyptian infants and toddlers on the bayley-III scales according to age and sex
Italian Journal of Pediatrics volume 50, Article number: 68 (2024)
Abstract
Background
Child development is shaped throughout the first years of life through the interaction of genetics and the environment. Bayley-III is valuably used to determine early developmental delay (DD). The aim of this study was to detect the differences in performance of a sample of apparently healthy Egyptian infants and toddlers on the Bayley-III scales in relation to their age and gender.
Methods
This was a cross-sectional study. Bayley scales were applied to 270 of the 300 recruited children following the inclusion criteria; to avoid potential risk factors affecting development. Assessment included cognitive, language and motor skills. Engaged children aged 18–42 months were divided into 4 age groups with six-month intervals.
Results
Approximately 78.4%, 76.2%, and 72% of the participants had average and above average scores in the cognitive, motor, and language domains, respectively. The language domain was characteristically impacted. The oldest age group (36–42 months) scored the highest means composite scores, while the 2nd group aged 24 - <30 months, scored the lowest means in the three evaluated domains. In general, girls had non-significantly higher composite scores than boys, with a small effect size (d = 0.2–0.4). In the language domain, girls aged 30 to < 36 months scored significantly higher composite scores than boys (p < 0.05), with a medium effect size (d = 0.73).
Conclusion
The study indicates that the performance of apparently healthy Egyptian children on the Bayley III evaluation differs in relation to age and sex. The most vulnerable age group at potential risk of DD was children aged 24–30 months. Efforts must be directed to investigate the nutritional, physical, psychological and safety needs of this group. Attention must be paid to early childhood intervention programs that stimulate development, especially language development, and they must be tailored on the basis of age and gender. Gender-specific norms may be needed in the evaluation of language development.
Introduction
The development of children is shaped during the first years of life, which lays the foundations for productivity and well-being later in life. However, this period is also considered to be the most vulnerable period [1]. The quality of the childrearing environment is critical for healthy development. A safe nurturing environment, positive interactions and sharing time with parents are in a positive direct relation to the child’s development [2]. Negative influences such as poverty, poor nutrition, under-stimulating environments and unhealthy surroundings can impede and restrain development [3]. It has been found that the prevalence of childhood delay varies from 10% in Central Asia and Europe to 42% in Central and West Africa [4]. In the USA, developmental delay was found to be 17% more common between the ages of 3 and 17 in boys than in girls [5]. In Egypt, a national community-based study proved that the prevalence of developmental delays in Egyptian children aged 1–12 years is approximately 6.7%, with a higher incidence among boys than girls [6].
During stages of child development, children go through several changes in different domains of physical, communication/language, social/emotional, and intellectual/cognitive development. Particular changes that occur at specific ages of life are called developmental milestones [7].
The lack of information about the common age at which healthy children attain different developmental milestones and whether milestones are attained similarly across sexes remains a principal barrier to defining developmental difficulties and starting intervention efforts in low- and middle-income countries [8].
The developmental assessment of infants is a complicated and time-consuming process. Standardized tools provide a means of evaluating a young child’s development and comparing this to a standardized norm [9]. The Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development 3rd Edition (Bayley-III), with scales for cognition, motor and language development, provides a gold standard developmental assessment tool in clinical and research fields to assess development in infancy and early childhood (0–42 months) [10]. The Bayley III normative population was an American sample of children who were stratified into different age groups, but the test lacked separate gender-specific norms [10].
Some studies have raised concerns about the underestimation of Bayley-III in identifying children with developmental delay compared to Bayley-II [11,12,13]. However, these studies were mainly conducted in premature infants and specific age ranges. The results from the Bayley scales must be interpreted carefully for all age ranges and in different contexts, taking into consideration the sociodemographic and cultural differences from the standardized American norms.
Neuroscience research has reported sex-based differences in brain structure and function induced by the action of gonadal sex hormones or genes found on sex chromosomes [14]. The National Institute of Mental Health recommended that sex should be incorporated as a significant variable in experimental and clinical studies [15]. The performance of boys and girls may be affected by sex differences in neuronal structures, neurochemistry, neuroanatomy and connectivity [16]. Previous research on sex differences in early childhood development is inconclusive; some studies have revealed gender differences in a number of functions or behaviors [17, 18]. A recent national Egyptian study showed that boys were 1.75 times more likely than girls to be diagnosed with any developmental delays (OR = 1.75, CI: 1.61–1.89) [6], and other research did not find any effect of gender [19]. These differences may be due to variances in methodological issues. Some of the psychological tests other than Bayley-III comprise gender-specific subgroup norms, such as the Language Development Survey [20] and the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories [21].
The aim of this study was to detect the differences in performance of a sample of apparently healthy Egyptian infants and toddlers on Bayley scales in relation to age and gender. Accordingly, attention can be directed with more efforts from caregivers, early childhood facilities and governmental institutions toward the most vulnerable age group and gender.
Subjects and methods
Sample size
The sample size was ascertained utilizing epi Info-Statcalc version 7 [22]. The power of this study to calculate the needed sample size was set at 80%, denoting the probability of finding a difference when a difference exists. A sample size of 240 infants and toddlers was estimated. This number was estimated to calculate a true difference of 15% (denoting the probability of finding a difference when no difference existed, i.e., margin of error: ± 0.15) [23]. Then, 10% was added for the expected losses. Determination of the sample size depended on the previously estimated prevalence of developmental delay in different countries, which varied from 10% in Central Asia and Europe to 42% in Central and West Africa [4]. This great variability provided the basis for the sample size calculation, which ensured the largest sample size for detecting any significant difference.
Study design and time frame
This was a cross-sectional study in which three hundred children were recruited from September 2017 to September 2019, and Bayley scales were applied to 270 of them following the inclusion and exclusion criteria. Engaged children aged 18 to 42 months were divided into 4 age groups with six-month intervals.
Selection criteria
To avoid the effect of potential risk factors affecting child development, Egyptian infants and toddlers were included according to the following exclusion and inclusion criteria. Children with reported prematurity or low birth weight, genetic, congenital, or metabolic disorders, a history of perinatal complications such as intracranial hemorrhage, a history of chronic disease, or severe sensory impairment (auditory or visual) were excluded. Additionally, the child was excluded if he/she was severely malnourished at the time of recruitment (if the height-per age z score (HAZ), weight-per-age z score (WAZ) or body mass index per age z score (BAZ) was less than − 2). Accordingly, infants and toddlers whose gestational age was at least 37 weeks, with a birth weight of at least 2500 g, infants and toddlers who had no physical or mental health issues and subjects who did not use medicine regularly or had chronic disease were included.
Ethical issues
The study proposal was approved by the Medical Research Ethical Committee of the National Research Centre, which complies with the International Ethical Guidelines for Biomedical Research Involving Human Subjects [24]. This study was a part of a project of the 11th research plan of the National Research Centre under the title of “A Pilot Study to Evaluate the Performance of Egyptian Infants on the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development -third edition (Bayley III)” with ID number 11,010,141.
Mothers or caregivers were informed about the purpose of the study, and their permission in the form of written consent was obtained.
Confidentiality: Mothers and children were identified by a serial number, and the information at the individual level was kept strictly confidential.
Recruitment of infants was from
-
The nursery that belongs to the National Research Centre (NRC).
-
Other nearby nurseries.
-
The Medical Research Centre of Excellence’s Clinics.
Setting
The study was conducted at the Developmental and Behavioral Assessment Clinic at the Medical Research Centre of Excellence, National Research Centre (NRC), Egypt.
Measurements
Background questionnaire
Mothers/caregivers answered a background questionnaire about family sociodemographic data and child characteristics.
Physical examination and growth assessment
Infants and toddlers were thoroughly examined by expert pediatricians. Growth assessment was performed using anthropometric measurements, including weight (kg), height (cm), and head circumference (cm). The length or height was measured to the nearest 0.5 cm, and the weight was measured to the nearest 0.1 kg using a scale balance with the subject dressed in minimal clothes. Body mass index (BMI) was calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared. Head circumference (HC) was measured (cm) around the child’s head to the nearest 0.1 cm. It is the maximum circumference passing around the glabella and the occiput. Each measurement was taken as the mean of three consecutive measurements using standardized equipment and following the recommendation of the International Biological Program [25]. WAZ, HAZ and BMI Z scores were calculated based on the WHO growth standards [26] with the help of the Anthro-Program of PC.
Assessment of development
Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (Bayley III), developed by Nancy Bayley in 2006, were utilized to assess the development of infants and toddlers between the age range of 1 month and 42 months [10]. The Bayley scales are described as the most widely used developmental assessment scheme [27]. Bayley-III covers five developmental domains. Cognitive, motor and language tests are administered with the child; interaction, social-emotional and adaptive behavior tests are administered with parent questionnaires. All domain subtests can be administered individually. In the current study, only the cognitive, motor and language domains were assessed directly.
The Cognitive Scale included items that assess sensorimotor development, exploration and manipulation, object relatedness, concept formation, memory, and other aspects of cognitive processing.
The Language Scale is composed of receptive communication and expressive communication items. The receptive communication subtest included items that assess preverbal behaviors, vocabulary development and children’s social referencing and verbal comprehension. The Expressive Communication subtest included items that assess preverbal communication, such as babbling, gesturing, joint referencing, and turn-taking, and vocabulary development, such as naming objects, pictures, and attributes (e.g., color and size).
The Motor Scale is divided into the Fine Motor subtest and the Gross Motor subtest. Fine motor skills included items that measure skills related to visual tracking, reaching, object manipulation, and grasping. The Gross Motor subtest assessed static positioning (e.g., sitting, standing), dynamic movement (e.g., locomotion, coordination), balance, and motor planning.
Scoring for every item is either 1 (credit) or 0 (no credit). Scores available include raw scores, scaled scores, composite scores, percentile ranks and confidence intervals.
The measure with a series of developmental play tasks took between 45 and 60 min to administer. Raw scores of successfully completed items were converted to scaled scores and composite scores. The scores obtained by toddlers were used to determine their performance compared with norms taken from typically developing children. The composite scores are scaled to a metric with a mean of 100, SD of 15 and a range from 40 to 160. The norm-referenced average is from 85 to 115 [10].
Statistical analysis
Data analysis was performed using Statistical Package for the Social Science (SPSS) version 21 (SSPS Inc., Pennsylvania, USA). Continuous data are expressed as the mean ± SD, while categorical data are expressed as frequencies and percentages. ANOVA was used to analyse the significant differences between the mean scores of the age groups. The P value was considered statistically significant at p < 0.05. Effect sizes were reported as either Cohen’s d or eta squared (η2) for t tests and ANOVAs, respectively. The mean (standard deviation) of the original normative Bayley population was 100 ± 15. Infants were considered below average if a Bayley III score was below 85 on any of the language, cognitive, or motor scales.
Results
The total number of participants was 270 infants and toddlers. The sociodemographic features of the studied children, including their gender, age and social class, are shown in Table 1. Most of the children were from the middle social class (77.6%), the high social class represented 16.3% and the low social class represented 6.1% of the whole sample.
Table 2 illustrates the body mass index of the participants according to the z score; it shows that most of the participants had normal BMI-z scores (92.6%), obese children were approximately 5.3%, and underweight children were only 2.1%.
The participants were classified according to the cut-off point of the composite score of the Bayley Scale domains into two groups: one group had average and above average scores (i.e., scores equal to or above 85), while the other group had below average scores (i.e., below 85). We found that approximately 78.4%, 72%, and 76.2% of the participants had average and above-average scores in the cognitive, language and motor domains, respectively (Table 3).
The effect of gender
It is observed that female participants in the whole sample had higher mean composite scores in the three domains than males by 3–4 points, but the differences were statistically insignificant. (94.41 ± 20.56 vs. 91.08 ± 16.53 in the cognitive domain; 93.89 ± 15.97 vs. 90.06 ± 15.15 in the language domain; 97.87 ± 19.77 vs. 93.96 ± 18.06 in the motor domain; p > 0.05 in all domains).
Table 4 shows a comparison of the mean composite scores according to gender. Girls had non-significantly higher scores than boys, with a small effect size (d = 0.2–0.4) in all domains in different age ranges, except the age range from 24 to 30 months, where boys scored higher in all domains. Girls had significantly higher scores with a medium effect size (d = 0.73) in the language composite score in the age range from 30 to 36 months.
In addition, on classification of participants according to the cut-off point, the girls had lower percentages of below-average performers than boys, particularly in the language domain, but the difference was not statistically significant (Fig. 1).
The effect of age
In the present study, participants were classified into 4 age groups, 6 months apart starting from 18 months to 42 months. The mean composite scores of the cognitive, language, and motor domains among the different age categories were compared.
Table 5 shows that the participants in the age range 24 months to < 30 months (group 2) had the lowest composite scores in all domains (cognitive, language and motor). The participants in the age range of 36 months to 42 months (group 4) had the highest composite scores in all domains. There are highly significant differences in cognitive composite scores between different ages.
with medium effect sizes. Additionally, there were highly significant differences in motor composite scores between different age groups with large effect sizes. Meanwhile, the differences in the language composite scores between different age groups were insignificant with small effect sizes.
These results directed attention towards toddlers aged 24–30 months who looked at the potential risk for developmental delay in motor and cognitive development.
Discussion
The Bayley-III affords a substantial benefit by providing differential diagnostic information when there are preliminary suspicions of developmental disorders with independent standard scores in the different scales and subtests (cognitive, language, receptive communication, expressive communication, fine motor, gross motor) [28, 29]. In this study, we assessed the performance of apparently healthy Egyptian infants and toddlers on the Bayley-III scales in relation to age and gender.
Within the related literature, there are many national and international studies of various facets of child development using Bayley-III in the evaluation process or as a predictive tool [30,31,32,33,34,35].
In the current study, it was found that girls had higher scores than boys in all domains, and they had a lower percentage of below-average population. However, the differences were not statistically significant, with a small effect size. In the language domain, girls aged 30 to < 36 months scored significantly higher than boys (p < 0.05), with a medium effect size (d = 0.73).
Very few studies have explored gender differences in test scores. Nationally, this study is the first to analyse the gender effect on infants’ performance on the Bayley-III scales. Internationally, many studies have proven the superiority of girls. Wu et al., 2008 [36] reported that female gender was only associated with higher mental and motor scores on Bayley scales. Another study stated that females scored significantly higher on the cognitive, receptive and expressive communication subtests [37]. Moreover, another study found that females scored higher on the fine motor subtest [38]. There are theories that have attempted to explain this gender difference, involving biological and social factors. According to biological theory, sex-based differences in brain structure and physiology reflect gonadal hormone/receptor interactions, their effects within cells, and the intermediating impact of genetic variables, principally the holding of an XX versus an XY genotype [14]. The greater risk of DD in boys is recognized to be linked to hereditary factors in the form of X-linked disorders and the related effects on the central nervous system [39].
Regarding social factors, especially in Asian and African nations, males are still the preferred gender, acquiring more attention and care added to their hasty and aggressive behaviors, making them more liable to diagnosis than females [40, 41].
Predomination of girls on boys in language development was observed in the current study, which is on the same track as earlier studies using Bayley-III or other developmental assessment tests. These studies have shown that girls tend to attain a variety of linguistic skills, such as language comprehension, gesturing expression, vocabulary and the capability to combine words, earlier than boys [42,43,44,45]. It has been found that parents talk more and in a different way to their daughters than to their sons using a supportive conversation to them, which in turn affords daughters potentially allowing for greater exposure to language [46]. Additionally, one study reported that parental play with girls is unlike that with boys; parents prefer symbolic play with daughters while with sons it is an action-oriented one, which in turn affects both quantity and excellence of language used [47]. In addition, gender distinctions were linked not only to differences in the measured functions but also to differences in the behavior of taking this test between girls and boys. In other words, girls have a better capability to control impulses and attention, while boys are more energetic and active [43, 48].
In this study, we thoroughly investigated the effect of age. It was found that those children in the younger age group (those younger than the age of 30 months) tended to have lower scores than older age groups, with highly significant differences between age groups. The highest composite scores of all subtests and the lowest percentage of below-average children were recorded in the oldest age group (from 36 to 42 months). These outcomes are consistent with Hanlon et al., 2016 [49]. They found that the mean score for each Bayley subscale and the total Bayley Scale were significantly lower in 30-month-old children than in 42-month-old children. The same results were observed by Steenis et al., 2015 [50], but in their study, they related the findings of the scaled scores to the mother’s educational level. They showed that with increasing age, children of mothers with higher levels of education had superior scores on the cognition and receptive communication subtests compared to children of mothers with lower levels of education.
In Egypt, multiple social, economic and nutritional factors could impact the development of young children in several domains. Children aged under 30 months may be constrained within their home environment, lacking the influences of outdoor stimulants. In addition, due to economic causes, Egyptian children may have received fewer opportunities to explore new things, such as toys and picture books, at a young age compared to children in other countries. Most of the mothers in developing countries showed a lack of knowledge on the proper timing of providing different stimulation activities [51, 52]. In addition, a lack of exclusive breastfeeding, as well as malnutrition, can arise, resulting in the risk of differences in social behavior and cognitive and motor development [53, 54]. Gunardi et al., 2019 [55] confirmed that nutritional problems have been linked to developmental delay. A cross-sectional Nigerian study proved a significant association between weight and language and interactive social domains [56]. A meta-analysis study by Sudfeld et al., 2015 [57] found that malnutrition leads to delays in maturation of the auditory system, difficulty in understanding information, apathy and delayed social interaction skills.
Although the majority of participants in the current study had normal physical growth, they still may have had micronutrient deficiency, particularly if they had experienced inappropriate weaning procedures. Deficiencies in iron, zinc, vitamin B12, folate, vitamin A, vitamin D, and iodine can each have severe consequences, including increased susceptibility to infections, reduced growth, cognitive impairment, and decreased school performance in older children [58]. Most micronutrient deficiencies remain undiagnosed due to ambiguous symptoms. However, approximately 56% of preschool children aged 6–59 months worldwide have one or more micronutrient deficiencies [59]. Many national and international studies have confirmed the association between low serum levels of micronutrients and below-average developmental scores [3, 60,61,62].
In the current study, the mean composite score in the language domain was lower than scores in the cognitive or motor domains. It seems that language skills may be particularly impacted in Egyptian and Arab children due to authoritarian parenting styles [50]. Frequent use of commands and criticisms in the early years of life [63], together with a lack of parental responsiveness and support, are associated with delays in children’s language abilities [49,50,51].
Some limitations were noted in this study. The majority of participants belonged to one social class (middle class), so the effect of socioeconomic status on the performance of participants could not be elicited. In addition, there was limited time to study parent‒child relationships and interactions due to the extensive time needed to conduct developmental assessments using the Bayley scales. Studying these variables could have provided an increased understanding of the dynamics and their association with the results. Despite these limitations, the strength of this study was that it was the first study performed in Egypt to discuss the association between age, gender and developmental status to prompt appropriate intervention strategies to support infants and children. This study could be the starting point for more detailed research that would include a larger number of participants, allowing increased generalization.
Conclusion and recommendations
This study indicates that the performance of apparently healthy Egyptian children on the Bayley III scale differs in relation to age and sex. The most vulnerable age group at potential risk of DD was children aged 24–30 months. Efforts must be directed to investigate and provide nutritional, physical, psychological and safety needs for this group. Proper feeding strategies and dietary diversification for pregnant mothers, infants and young children should be practiced. Fortified foods with an appropriate content of micronutrients should be available, especially to low-income families. Healthcare providers must be sufficiently knowledgeable about child developmental milestones in different age stages and take the initiative to provide precise information to primary caregivers. Attention must be paid to early childhood intervention programs that stimulate development, especially language development, and they must be tailored on the basis of age and gender. Gender-specific norms may be needed in the evaluation of language development.
Data availability
The datasets used and analysed during the current study are fully available without restriction upon request from Mones M. Abushady, monesshady@ymail.com.
Abbreviations
- DD:
-
developmental delay
- LMICs:
-
Low- and middle-income countries
- HAZ:
-
height-per age z score
- WAZ:
-
weight-per age z score
- BAZ:
-
body mass index per age z score
- NRC:
-
National Research Centre
- BMI:
-
Body mass index
References
Hoskens J, Klingels K, Smits-Engelsman B. Validity and cross-cultural differences of the Bayley scales of Infant and Toddler Development, Third Edition in typically developing infants. Early Hum Dev. 2018;125:17–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2018.07.002.
Sawada Y, Katayama T. Characteristics of child-rearing environments related to Social Development in Early Childhood. Child (Basel). 2022;12(6):877. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9060877.
El Din EMS, Rabah TM, Metwally AM, Nassar MS, Elabd MA, Shalaan A, Kandeel W, El Etreby LA, Shaaban SY. Potential risk factors of Developmental Cognitive Delay in the first two years of life. Open Access Maced J Med Sci. 2019;7(12):2024–30. https://doi.org/10.3889/oamjms.2019.566.
Gil JD, Ewerling F, Ferreira LZ, Barros AJ. Early childhood suspected developmental delay in 63 low- and middle-income countries: large within- and between-country inequalities documented using national health surveys. J Glob Health. 2020;10(1):010427. https://doi.org/10.7189/jogh.10.010427. PMID: 32566165; PMCID: PMC7295453.
Zablotsky B, Black LI, Maenner MJ, Schieve LA, Danielson ML, Bitsko RH, Blumberg SJ, Kogan MD, Boyle CA. Prevalence and Trends of Developmental Disabilities among children in the United States: 2009–2017. Pediatrics. Oct; 2019;144(4):e20190811. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2019-0811.
Metwally AM, Abdallah AM, Salah El-Din EM, Khadr Z, Raouf ERA, Elghareeb NA, Saleh RM, Abuelela MH, Amer HA, Hasanin HM, Mawla MAA, Sallam SF, El-Alameey IR, Sami SM, Abdel-Latif GA, Abdelrahman M, Shehata MA. A national prevalence and profile of single and multiple developmental delays among children aged from 1 year up to 12 years: an Egyptian community-based study. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2022;5(1):63. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-022-00498-3. PMID: 35932037; PMCID: PMC9356393.
Improving early childhood development. WHO guideline. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2020.
Ertem IO, Krishnamurthy V, Mulaudzi MC, Sguassero Y, Balta H, Gulumser O, Bilik B, Srinivasan R, Johnson B, Gan G, Calvocoressi L, Shabanova V, Forsyth BWC. Similarities and differences in child development from birth to age 3 years by sex and across four countries: a cross-sectional, observational study. Lancet Global Health. 2018;6(3):e279–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2214-109X(18)30003-2.
Center for Disease Control and Prevention. cdc.gov. Screening and Diagnosis for Healthcare Providers. http://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/autism/hcp-screening.html.
Bayley N. Bayley scales of infant and toddler development, third edition: technical manual. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt; 2006.
Çelik P, Ayranci Sucakli I, Yakut HI. Which Bayley-III cut-off values should be used in different developmental levels? Turk J Med Sci. 2020;50:764–70. https://doi.org/10.3906/sag-1910-69.
Bos AF. Bayley-II or Bayley-III: what do the scores tell us? Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55(11):978–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/dmcn.12234.
Anderson PJ, De Luca CR, Hutchinson E, Roberts G. Doyle LW Underestimation of developmental delay by the new Bayley-III Scale. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010;164:352.
Zhang X, Liang M, Qin W, Wan B, Yu C, Ming D. Gender differences are encoded differently in the structure and function of the human brain revealed by Multimodal MRI. Front Hum Neurosci. 2020;14:244. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.00244.
National Institute of Mental Health. Sex differences in brain, behavior, mental health and mental disorders, Workshop. Bethesda, Md, USA, February 2011; http://www.nimh.nih.gov/research-priorities/scientific-meetings/2011/sex-differences-inbrain-behavior-mental-health-and-mental-disorders/index.shtml.
Zagni E, Simoni L, Colombo D. Sex and gender differences in Central Nervous System-Related disorders. Neurosci J. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/2827090.
Månsson J, Fellman V, Stjernqvist K. Extremely preterm birth affects boys more and socioeconomic and neonatal variables pose sex-specific risks. Acta Pædiatrica. 2015;104(5):514–21.
Chen JJ. Gender differences in externalizing problems among preschool children: implications for early childhood educators. Early Child Dev Care. 2010;180(4):463–74.
Kaplan PS, Danko CM, Everhart KD, Diaz A, Asherin RM, Vogeli JM, Fekri SM. Maternal depression and expressive communication in one year-old infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2014;37(3):398–405.
Rescorla L. The Language Development Survey: a screening tool for delayed language in toddlers. J Speech Hear Disord. 1989;54(4):587–99.
Fenson L, Marchman VA, Thal DJ, Dale PS, Reznick JS, Bates E. MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories. User’s guide and technical manual (second edition). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co; 2007.
Kelsey JL, Whittemore AS, Evans AS, Thompson WD. Methods of sampling and estimation of sample size. In: Kelsey JL, Whittemore AS, Evans AS, Thompson WD, editors. Methods in Observational Epidemiology. New York: Oxford University Press; 1996.
Hahn GJ, Meeker WQ. (1991) Statistical Intervals: A Guide for Practitioners, John Wiley & Sons, New York.1991; https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470316771.
CIOMS/WHO. International ethical guidelines for Biomedical Research Involving human subjects. Geneva: CIOMS; 2002.
Tanner JM. Physical growth and development. In: Forfar JO, Arnell CC, editors. Textbook of pediatrics. 2nd ed. Scotland: Churchill Livingstone; 1998. pp. 249–303.
World Health Organization. WHO child growth standards: growth velocity based on weight, length and head circumference: methods and development. World Health Organ. 2009; https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/44026.
Moore T, Johnson S, Haider S, Hennessy E, Marlow N. Relationship between test scores using the second and third editions of the Bayley scales in extremely preterm children. J Pediatr. 2012;160:553–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.peds.2011.09.047.
Yi YG, Sung IY, Yuk JS. Comparison of second and third editions of the Bayley scales in children with suspected Developmental Delay. Ann Rehabil Med. 2018;42(2):313–20. https://doi.org/10.5535/arm.2018.42.2.313.
Del Rosario C, Slevin M, Molloy EJ. How to use the Bayley scales of Infant and Toddler Development. Archives Disease Child - Educ Pract. 2021;106:108–12. https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2020-319063. Quigley J, Nixon E.
Andrew MJ, Parr JR, Montague-Johnson C, Laler K, Holmes J, Baker B, Sullivan PB. Neurodevelopmental outcome of nutritional intervention in newborn infants at risk of neurodevelopmental impairment: the Dolphin neonatal double-blind randomized controlled trial. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2018;60(9):897–905. https://doi.org/10.1111/dmcn.13914.
Bogičević L, Verhoeven M, van Baar AL. Toddler skills predict moderate-to-late preterm born children’s cognition and behavior at 6 years of age. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(11):e0223690. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223690.
El-Din EMS, Elabd MA, Nassar MS, Metwally AM, Abdellatif GA, Rabah TM, Shalaan A, Shaaban SY, Kandeel W, Etreby LAE, Al-Tohamy M. The Interaction of Social, Physical and Nutritive Factors in triggering early Developmental Language Delay in a sample of Egyptian children. Open Access Maced J Med Sci. 2019;7(17):2767–74. https://doi.org/10.3889/oamjms.2019.642.
Sadhwani A, Wypij D, Rofeberg V, Gholipour A, Mittleman M, Rohde J, Velasco-Annis C, Calderon J, Friedman KG, Tworetzky W, Grant PE, Soul JS, Warfield SK, Newburger JW, Ortinau CM, Rollins CK. Fetal brain volume predicts neurodevelopment in congenital heart disease. Circulation. 2022;145:1108–19. https://doi.org/10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.121.056305.
Liang X, Zhang X, Wang Y, van IJzendoorn MH, Wang Z. Sleep problems and infant motor and cognitive development across the first two years of life: the Beijing Longitudinal Study. Infant Behav Dev. 2022;66:101686. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101686.
Godamunne P, Liyanage C, Wimaladharmasooriya N, Pathmeswaran A, Wickremasinghe AR, Patterson, Sathiakumar N. (2014). Comparison of performance of Sri Lankan and US children on cognitive and motor scales of the Bayley scales of infant development. BMC Res Notes. 2014; 7, 300. https://doi.org/10.1186/1756-0500-7-300.
Wu Y, Tsou K, Hsu C, Fang L, Yao G, Jeng S. Taiwanese infants’ mental and motor development 6–24 months. J Pediatr Psychol. 2008;33(1):102–8. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsm067.
Koutra K, Chatzi L, Roumeliotaki T, Vassilaki M, Giannakopoulou E, Batsos C, Koutis A, Kogevinas M. Sociodemographic determinants of infant neurodevelopment at 18 months of age: mother-child cohort (Rhea Study) in Crete, Greece. Infant Behav Dev. 2012;35(1):48–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.09.005.
Suhonen E, Sajaniemi NK, Alijoki A, Nislin MA. (2018). Children’s biological givens, stress responses, language and cognitive abilities and family background after entering kindergarten in toddlerhood. Early Child Development and Care. 2018; 188(3), 345–358https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2016.1218157.
Tseng Y-C, Lai D-C, Guo H-R. Gender and geographic differences in the prevalence of reportable childhood speech and language disability in Taiwan. Res Dev Disabil. 2015;40:11–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2015.01.009.
Lai DC, Tseng YC, Guo HR. Characteristics of young children with developmental delays and their trends over 14 years in Taiwan: a population-based nationwide study. BMJ Open. 2018;8(5):e020994. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2017-020994.
Radi S. Gender inequality and its impact on Early Childhood Development, Health and Education in the Democratic Republic of Congo. IJISRT. 2020;5:472–80.
Krogh MT, Væver MS. Does gender affect Bayley-III scores and test-taking behavior? Infant Behav Dev. 2019;57:101352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2019.101352.
Kung KT, Browne WV, Constantinescu M, Noorderhaven RM, Hines M. Early postnatal testosterone predicts sex-related differences in early expressive vocabulary. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2016;68:111–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.03.001.
Hollier LP, Mattes E, Maybery MT, Keelan JA, Hickey M, Whitehouse AJ. The association between perinatal testosterone concentration and early vocabulary development: a prospective cohort study. Biol Psychol. 2013;92(2):212–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.10.016.
Schild CE, Meigen C, Kappelt J, Kiess W, Poulain T. Associations between sociodemographic and behavioural parameters and child development depending on age and sex: a cross-sectional analysis. BMJ Open. 2022;12(11):e065936. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2022-065936. PMID: 36323480; PMCID: PMC9639104.
Rinaldi P, Pasqualetti P, Volterra V, Caselli MC. Gender differences in early stages of language development. Some evidence and possible explanations. J Neurosci Res. 2021 Jul 9. https://doi.org/10.1002/jnr.24914. Epub ahead of print. PMID: 34240751.
Bleses D, Vach W, Dale PS. Self-reported parental vocabulary input frequency for young children. J Child Lang. 2018;45(5):1073–90. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000918000089. [Opens in a new window].
Gagne JR, Miller MM, Goldsmith HH. Early but modest gender differences in focal aspects of childhood temperament. Pers Indiv Differ. 2013;55(2):95–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.02.006.
Hanlon C, Medhin G, Worku B, Tomlinson M, Alem A, Dewey M, Prince M. Adapting the Bayley scales of infant and toddler development in Ethiopia: evaluation of reliability and validity: measuring child development in Ethiopia. Child Care Health Dev. 2016;42(5):699–708. https://doi.org/10.1111/cch.12371.
Steenis LJ, Verhoeven M, Hessen DJ, van Baar AL. Performance of Dutch children on the Bayley III: a comparison study of US and Dutch norms. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(8):e0132871. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0132871.
Shrestha M, Ulak M, Strand TA, Kvestad I, Hysing M. How much do Nepalese mothers know about child development? Early Child Dev Care. 2017;189:135–42. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2017.1304391.
Munoz-Chereau B, Ang L, Dockrell J, Outhwaite L, Heffernan C. Measuring early child development across low and middle-income countries: a systematic review. J Early Child Res. 2021;19(4):443470. https://doi.org/10.1177/1476718X211020031.
Tanner J, Candland T, Odden W. Later impacts of early childhood interventions: A systematic review. IEG Working Papers. Washington DC: World Bank Group. 2015; pp. 47– 9.
Kandeel WA, Rabah TM, Zeid DA, El-Din EMS, Metwally AM, Shaalan A, El Etreby LA, Shaaban SY. Determinants of exclusive breastfeeding in a sample of Egyptian infants. Open Access Maced J Med Sci. 2018;6(10):1818–23. https://doi.org/10.3889/oamjms.2018.359.
Gunardi H, Nugraheni R, Yulman A, Soedjatmiko S, Sekartini R, Medise B, Wirahmadi A, Melina E. Growth and developmental delay risk factors among underfive children in an inner-city slum area. PI [Internet]. 2019;59(5):276–3. https://doi.org/10.14238/pi59.5.2019.276-83.
Jimoh AO, Anyiam JO, Yakubu AM. Relationship between child development and nutritional status of underfive Nigerian children. South Afr J Clin Nutr. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1080/16070658.2017.1387434. 31, 50– 4.
Sudfeld CR, Charles MD, Danaei G, Fink G, Ezzati M, Andrews KG, et al. Linear growth and child development in low- and middle-income countries: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2015;135:1266–75. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2014-3111.
Bailey RL, West KP Jr, Black RE. The epidemiology of global micronutrient deficiencies. Ann Nutr Metab. 2015;66(suppl 2):22–33. https://doi.org/10.1159/000371618.
Stevens GA, Beal T, Mbuya MNN, Luo H, Neufeld LM, Global Micronutrient Deficiencies Research Group. Lancet Glob Health. 2022;10(11):e1590–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2214-109X(22)00376-9. Micronutrient deficiencies among preschool-aged children and women of reproductive age worldwide: a pooled analysis of individual-level data from population-representative surveys.
Metwally AM, Salah El- Din EM, Shehata MA, Shaalan A, El Etreby LA, Kandeel WA, et al. Early life predictors of Socio-Emotional Development in a sample of Egyptian infants. PLoS ONE. 2016;11(7):e0158086. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0158086.
Petry N, Olofin I, Hurrell RF, Boy E, Wirth JP, Moursi M, Donahue Angel M, Rohner F. The proportion of anemia associated with iron deficiency in low, medium, and high human development index countries: a systematic analysis of national surveys. Nutrients. 2016;8:693. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu8110693.
Black MM. Zinc deficiency and child development. Am J Clin Nutr. 1998;68(suppl 2):S464–69. https://doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/68.2.464S.
Garcia D, Bagner DM, Pruden SM, Nichols-Lopez K. Language production in children with and at risk for Delay: mediating role of parenting skills. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2015;44(5):814–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2014.900718.
Acknowledgements
The authors express their deep appreciation to the officials in the nursery held by the National Research Centre, the adjacent nurseries, and the Medical Research Centre of Excellence who facilitated the recruitment of the children. Special thanks to all caregivers for their willing participation and cooperation during the implementation of the study.
Funding
This study was conducted through a project titled “A Pilot Study to Evaluate the Performance of Egyptian Infants on the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development -third edition (Bayley III)” under the leadership of Prof.: Zeinab M. Monir. The project was supported financially by the National Research Centre of Egypt, project ID Number “11010141”. The funder had no role in the design of the study, collection, analysis, and interpretation of data or in writing the manuscript.
Open access funding provided by The Science, Technology & Innovation Funding Authority (STDF) in cooperation with The Egyptian Knowledge Bank (EKB).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
ZM, ESD, and DAZ wrote the main manuscript text, and MAS prepared the data for statistical analysis and designed the tables. ZM, ESD, WK, SS, EE, MAS, FHA, ST, and DAZ participated in the recruitment of patients and implementation of the study and reviewed the manuscript. All the authors have read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethical approval
The study proposal was approved by the Medical Research Ethical Committee of the National Research Centre; this study was a part of a project of the 11th research plan of the National Research Centre under the title of “A Pilot Study to Evaluate the Performance of Egyptian Infants on the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development -third edition (Bayley III)” with ID number 11010141. The study proposal was approved by the Medical Research Ethical Committee of the National Research Centre and complies with the International Ethical Guidelines for Biomedical Research Involving Human Subjects (CIOMS/WHO., 2002)19.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Informed consent
Mothers or caregivers were informed about the purpose of the study, and their permission in the form of written consent was obtained. Confidentiality: Mothers and children were identified by a serial number, and the information at the individual level was kept strictly confidential. All our methods in this study were performed in accordance with the ethical standards of the Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Competing interests
All authors report no conflicts of interest.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Monir, Z.M., El-Din, E.M.S., Kandeel, W.A. et al. Evaluation of apparently healthy Egyptian infants and toddlers on the bayley-III scales according to age and sex. Ital J Pediatr 50, 68 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13052-024-01635-8
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13052-024-01635-8