Skip to main content

Assessing basic motor competences, physical fitness, and executive function in 4-5-year-old children: a longitudinal study in a primary care setting

Abstract

Background

To evaluate the progression of physical fitness (PF), basic motor competence (BMC), and executive function (EF) over one year in children aged 4–5 years at a health center.

Methods

In this longitudinal analysis, children’s BMC was evaluated using the MOBAK KG test for object and self-movement. The PREFIT Battery gauged PF through handgrip strength, standing long jump, and other fitness measures, while the Early Years Toolbox appraised EF.

Results

Adjustments for confounding factors showed notable improvements in BMC, particularly in object movement (OM; mean difference 0.789, p = 0.044) and self-movement (SM; mean difference 0.842, p = 0.037), with overall MOBAK scores also increasing (mean difference 1.632, p = 0.018). Enhancements in the standing long jump (mean difference 9.036 cm, p = 0.014) and EF tasks “Mr. Ant” (mean difference 0.669, p < 0.001) and “Go/No-Go” (mean difference 0.120, p < 0.001) were evident, signifying substantial BMC gains and some progress in PF and EF.

Conclusion

This research underscores the positive impact of regular training on BMC and PF in young children. Significant BMC development and associated improvements in PF and EF over the study period highlight the importance of structured activities in early childhood. These findings advocate for standardized training programs to enhance childhood health and encourage active lifestyles.

Trial registration

NCT05741879. Registered February 14, 2023, Version 1.

Background

Throughout normal child development, particularly in preschool-aged children, basic motor competencies (BMC), physical fitness (PF), and executive function (EF) concurrently develop [1]. Childhood is a crucial period for developing motor skills [2], where acquiring and developing BMC is an essential component for individuals’ physical, psychological and social growth and well-being throughout life [3]. Preschool BMC have been linked to health outcomes like adiposity [4], self-stem [5], cardiorespiratory capacity [6] and cognition [7]. During early childhood, progress in body control and mental processing abilities indicate overall development [8]. Encouraging BMC development in preschool is important because BMC directly connects to physical health, academic performance, and psychosocial well-being in children [9].

The preschool stage represents a vital period in children´s motor development where acquiring and refining BMC play a crucial role in overall development [10]. From a motor development perspective, BMC establishes an important basis for subsequently learning specific skills. Preschoolers undergo swift changes in motor coordination [11]. As children grow, they show progressive refinement in motor competence from accumulating motor experiences that spur motor control development [12,13,14].

It has commonly been assumed that PF signifies a powerful health marker in both childhood and adulthood, encompassing cardiovascular endurance, muscular strength and speed, all of which progressively increase during child development [1, 15]. On the other hand, EF includes cognitive skills that enable logical thinking, planning, problem-solving and life management [16]. EF has been linked to improved physical and mental health and performance in academic and everyday settings [17, 18].

Previous studies examined preschoolers in educational context [19, 20]. The novelty of our study involves the assessment of BMC, PF, and EF in 4-5-Year-Olds in a primary care center. We hypothesize that the population sample will be more heterogeneous in terms of socio-economic-cultural levels compared to previous studies. Assessing motor competence in a longitudinal primary care setting is vital for capturing the evolution of these skills where children frequently receive health check-ups, representing the general population and informing public health initiatives. The longitudinal design tracks individual progress, distinguishing typical development from potential delays or disorders, particularly crucial in early years when establishing BMC is strongly linked to subsequent health. Situating research in a real-world context ensures direct applicability of findings to healthcare providers, facilitating the implementation of evidence-based practices to promote healthy motor development.

In summary, evidence indicates positive associations between physical activity (PA), PF, cognition and academic achievement [19]. Cardiovascular capacity and muscular strength develop over time, typically showing improvements with growth and maturation [20]. Therefore, the aim of this study was to assess PF, BMC and EF in 4-year-old children and analyse their development one year later at 5 years of age in a primary care setting.

Methods

Study design and participants

A longitudinal study, part of the “Observatorio y programa de Intervención de ejercicio físico y estilos de vida en familia para niños y niñas de 4 a 5 años en Atención Primaria” project, conducted two assessments one year apart (2022–2023) and can be explored further at observatorioactividadfisica.es. The initial cohort comprised 70 children (38 boys, 32 girls) with an average age of 4.83 (± 0.49) years. Assessments were integrated into their routine primary care over the year, with 11 children failing to complete the second measurement—3 due to a change in their primary care center and 8 absent from the follow-up assessment. Families were informed of study purposes. Children provided oral assent and legal guardians written informed consent. Participants were 4-5-year-old children from the Iturrama primary care center in Pamplona, Spain, excluding recent injuries/surgeries or physical testing limitations or heart or respiratory system problems.

The protocol aligned with the Declaration of Helsinki following Ethics Committee of the Department of Health of Navarra approval (PI_2021/111).

Measures and procedures

The recruitment of participating families for the study was facilitated by the medical staff at the primary care centers. The personnel responsible for collecting PF data were professionals with expertise in PF and EF assessment. They received comprehensive training from the research staff of the coordinating center, the e-FIT UPNA Research Group.

Anthropometrics included height (cm), weight (kg), and body mass index (BMI) per CDC-NHANES protocols with trained assessors [21]. Height was measured in the Frankfurt position using a SECA 213® stadiometer (1-mm precision). Weight was measured with a Tanita DC-430MAS® scale (100-g precision) in light clothing without shoes. BMI was calculated as the ratio of an individual’s weight in kilograms divided by the height in meters squared (kg/m2). Waist circumference was measured to the nearest 1 mm at the umbilicus using a SECA 201 calibrated tape.

PF, encompassing cardiorespiratory fitness (CRF), lower and upper body strength, and speed-agility, was appraised using the PREFIT battery [21]. This battery is acknowledged as a practical and dependable means to evaluate physical fitness in preschool-aged children [22]. The circuit-based test was demonstrated then individually performed, except the 20 m in small 6-child subgroups. Upper limb strength was measured by hand grip with a Takei 5001® analog dynamometer squeezed for 2–3 s [23]. Children did two alternating hands with the higher value retained; their average comprised upper-body strength.

Lower limb strength was measured by the SLJ. Children performed a maximum horizontal jump from standing, landing on both feet while maintaining upright posture. Three attempts occurred with the best result (cm) recorded. The 4 × 10 m test assessed speed-agility. Children ran twice between two lines 10 m apart, covering 40 m total, with full recovery between attempts. The fastest time (seconds) was analyzed. CRF used the adapted 20 m shuttle run where children ran between lines 20 m apart. An audio signal of increasing pace (starting 6.5 km/h and elevating 0.5 km/h per minute) was used. The test ended when the child failed two consecutive attempts to reach a line or stopped due to exhaustion. Children performed one test with total laps recorded.

Standardized values (z-scores) were calculated per test by subtracting individual values from mean test values, then dividing the difference by the test standard deviation. Continuous scores were derived for each of the four selected fitness components separately for boys and girls. Higher PF z-scores indicate superior fitness.

The validated MOBAK KG test battery assessed BMC in this age group [24]. This 8-test battery measures preschooler (ages 4–6) object movement (OM) (throwing, catching, bouncing, dribbling) and self-movement (SM) (balancing, rolling, jumping, running) skills. Both subscales have maximum 8 points, producing a 0 (lowest) to 16 (highest) combined MOBAK score.

Participants performed tasks without prior attempts. “Throwing” and “catching” involved 6 attempts scoring: 0–2 attempts = 0 points; 3–4 attempts = 1 point; 5–6 attempts = 2 points. “Bouncing”, “dribbling”, “balancing”, “rolling”, “jumping” and “running” involved 2 attempts per task scored dichotomously (0 = fail, 1 = succeed), summed as: 0 points for 0 successes; 1 point for 1 success; 2 points for 2 successes. Identifying skill strengths/weaknesses informs targeted support areas.

EF assessment involved researchers administering digital iPad tests from the Early Years Toolbox TM (EYT-2017) comprising “Mr. Ant” and “Not This” for memory, “Card sorting” for cognitive flexibility, and “Go/No-Go” for inhibitory control. The Early Years Toolbox (EYT) is a collection of iPad measures assessing young children’s emerging cognitive, self-regulatory, language, numeracy, and social development through game-like assessments. EYT represents an advance over existing measures by capturing abilities shown predictive of later academic, social, emotional, cognitive and life outcomes. Researchers administered four EYT tasks: “Mr. Ant” and “Not This” for memory; “Card sorting” for cognitive flexibility; and “Go/No-Go” for inhibitory control.

Statistical analysis

Continuous variables were expressed as mean ± standard deviation. Normality was assessed using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, revealing significant BMC differences between time points in OM, SM, and total MOBAK score.

A repeated measures analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) examining anthropometric, BMC, PF, and EF differences was conducted, adjusting for age and BMI. The magnitude of the effect size was interpreted using thresholds as suggested by Cohen [43]: 0.0 to 0.19—trivial; 0.20 to 0.49—small; 0.50 to 0.79—moderate; >0.80—large. Analyses used IBM SPSS Statistics 26 with statistical significance set at p < 0.05.

Results

Table 1 presents descriptive characteristic summaries for the sample’s boys and girls, including anthropometric measurements, PF, BMC, and EF. No significant gender differences emerged. Table 2 shows PF, BMC, and EF differences between the two assessments one year apart. In year two, results improved significantly— SLJ (mean difference 9.036 cm, p = 0.014; d = 0.439); BMC OM (mean difference 0.789 points, p = 0.044; d = 0.353) and SM (mean difference 0.842 points, p = 0.037; d = 0.388); MOBAK total score (mean difference 1.632 points, p = 0.018; d = 0.418); and EF “Mr. Ant” (mean difference 0.669, p = < 0.001; d = 0.868) and “Go/No-Go” (mean difference 0.120, p = < 0.001; d = 0.667).

Table 1 Characteristics of the study population
Table 2 Comparison of anthropometric parameters, physical fitness, basic motor competences and executive function between 4 and 5-year-old children (one-year difference)

Figure 1 shows MOBAK results across the two assessments. In OM (Figure A), “catching” significantly improved (p = 0.006). In SM, “rolling” (p = 0.038) and “running” (p < 0.001) significantly improved. Figures B and C show significant improvements in OM (p = 0.044), SM (p = 0.037), and total score (p = 0.018).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Changes in Basic Motor Competences measured through the MOBAK KG battery. Time 1 (), Time 2 (■)

Discussion

The objective of this longitudinal study was to compare the one-year progression of PF, BMC and EF between boys and girls in a primary care setting. The main finding is the significant improvement in BMC and partial improvement in PF and EF. This study is significant due to the limited research evaluating BMC in children of these ages and due to the fact that the sample is obtained from a primary care center.

Overall, the one-year results were better than the baseline assessments. As literature shows, preschool-aged children constantly evolve motor, physical, and psychological capacities during development [25]. Developing BMC during childhood is essential for actively and successfully engaging in physical activities throughout life [26, 27]. An enhancement in proficiency of these skills at an early age could amplify the child´s perception of competence, giving to them the confidence to engage in sports later in life, thereby instigating a positive spiral of involvement from an early age [3].

Previous studies show boys tend to score higher in OM skills while girls demonstrate better balance and stability [28,29,30]. Our results align with this trend; boys achieved notably better BMC related to OM. These findings reflect those of Webster et al. showing superior OM performance in boys [31]. In SM skills, despite slightly higher girl scores, no significant differences emerged between boys and girls. One explanation may be the young age of our study children.

Due to typical growth and maturation, children´s BMC will naturally improve even in the absence of targeted interventions [32]. Nevertheless, the score obtained after one year of follow-up is close to the mean value of the MOBAK KG sum score. This indicates that our study population was below average (< 8 out of 16 points) initially, and after a year, despite statistically significant improvements, they still do not reach half of the total score.

We hypothesize that these low values may be attributed to low levels of PA. Several studies have documented the positive outcomes associated with PA [33]. For this reason, it is crucial not to underestimate the importance of adhering to the PA recommendations established by the World Health Organization [33].

We also observed improved EF, particularly on the “Mr. Ant” memory and Go/No-Go inhibitory control tests. These results support evidence that motor and cognitive development interconnect through common processes like sequencing, monitoring, and planning [34]. They also align with previous observations of a positive BMC and EF improvement association [35].

Regarding PF results, we observed significant SLJ test improvement. However, HGS and 20 m shuttle run PREFIT outcomes slightly decreased at the second measurement. These surprising findings contradict literature suggesting childhood PF improves over time [36]. Increased screen time and physical inactivity offer one explanation [37]. Still, interpret these results cautiously given our non-representative sample. Considering all results, we can hypothesize BMC acquisition precedes PF, as BMC improved more easily than PF. This underscores the importance of developing BMC to improve PF.

Existing literature has delineated the correlation between PF and BMC [28, 38, 39]. Studies focusing on BMC have consistently reported positive outcomes from a variety of training programs, ranging in duration from 2 to 10 months, with frequencies of 2–3 times per week, conducted in school or home settings [40]. A unique finding of these interventions is the successful enhancement of BMC among preschool-aged children. The review by Jones et al. further substantiates the value of reinforcing these foundational abilities, highlighting a beneficial link between them and early years physical activity (PA) [41]. Therefore, the promotion of BMC development and practice warrants more definitive guidelines and specialized training for educators. Future initiatives should aim to emphasize BMC skills training as a contribution to the bolstering of overall childhood PF and health. Given that BMC development is pivotal and consolidates during the early stages of life, laying this foundation is essential for encouraging ongoing engagement in PA [42].

A major strength of this study was using the MOBAK test battery to measure BMC in 4-5-year-olds an innovative approach given the dearth of BMC evidence in this population. Additionally, these findings are particularly remarkable because the recruitment was from a primary care center. The PREFIT tests deserve mention for their reliability and validity in assessing childhood PF.

Despite strengths, the one-year longitudinal design limits observations. Tracking children over several years would better elucidate development. Additionally, our non-representative sample size from one primary care center reduces generalizability. For comparable projects in the future, it is advisable to implement strategies aimed at minimizing the number of participants who drop out between assessments. Finally, Spanish BMC reference values for MOBAK tests in this age group are unavailable, globally complicating result comparisons. Diverse assessment tools and no measurement consensus highlight the need for an internationally standardized motor competence tool, an interesting research direction.

Conclusions

In conclusion, this one-year longitudinal study adds valuable BMC evidence in an under-researched population. Our results align with literature on BMC differences between boys and girls, as well as connections between developing BMC and EF. Partial PF improvements warrant further investigation given contradictory results. Standardizing BMC assessment and implementing early skills training offer important future research and practical directions for supporting childhood health.

Data availability

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study can be made available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

PA:

Physical activity

BMC:

Basic Motor Competences

BMI:

Body mass index

CRF:

Cardiorespiratory fitness

EF:

Executive Function

HGS:

Handgrip strength

KGF:

Kilograms of Force

MOBAK KG:

Motorsiche Basiskompetenzen Kindergarden

OM:

Object movement

PF:

Physical fitness

SLJ:

Standing long jump

SM:

Physical activity

References

  1. Niessner C, Utesch T, Oriwol D, Hanssen-Doose A, Schmidt SCE, Woll A, et al. Representative Percentile Curves of Physical Fitness from Early Childhood to early adulthood: the MoMo Study. Front Public Health. 2020;8:458.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Robinson LE, Stodden DF, Barnett LM, Lopes VP, Logan SW, Rodrigues LP, et al. Motor competence and its Effect on Positive Developmental Trajectories of Health. Sports Med. 2015;45(9):1273–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Stodden DF, Goodway JD, Langendorfer SJ, Roberton MA, Rudisill ME, Garcia C, et al. A developmental perspective on the role of Motor Skill competence in physical activity: an Emergent Relationship. Quest. 2008;60(2):290–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Okely AD, Booth ML, Chey T. Relationships between Body Composition and Fundamental Movement Skills among children and adolescents. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2004;75(3):238–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Ulrich BD. Perceptions of physical competence, motor competence, and participation in Organized Sport: their interrelationships in Young Children. Res Q Exerc Sport. 1987;58(1):57–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Okely AD, Booth ML, Patterson JW. Relationship of Cardiorespiratory Endurance to Fundamental Movement Skill proficiency among adolescents. Pediatr Exerc Sci. 2001;13(4):380–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Van Der Fels IMJ, Te Wierike SCM, Hartman E, Elferink-Gemser MT, Smith J, Visscher C. The relationship between motor skills and cognitive skills in 4–16 year old typically developing children: a systematic review. J Sci Med Sport. 2015;18(6):697–703.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Hernández Luengo M, Álvarez-Bueno C, Pozuelo-Carrascosa DP, Berlanga-Macías C, Martínez-Vizcaíno V, Notario-Pacheco B. Relationship between breast feeding and motor development in children: protocol for a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open. 2019;9(9):e029063.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Lubans DR, Morgan PJ, Cliff DP, Barnett LM, Okely AD. Fundamental Movement Skills in Children and Adolescents: review of Associated Health benefits. Sports Med. 2010;40(12):1019–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Wrotniak BH, Epstein LH, Dorn JM, Jones KE, Kondilis VA. The relationship between Motor proficiency and physical activity in children. Pediatrics. 2006;118(6):e1758–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Logan SW, Robinson LE, Wilson AE, Lucas WA. Getting the fundamentals of movement: a meta-analysis of the effectiveness of motor skill interventions in children. Child Care Health Dev. 2012;38(3):305–15.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Lester D, McGrane B, Belton S, Duncan M, Chambers F, O’Brien W. The Age-Related Association of Movement in Irish Adolescent Youth. Sports. 2017;5(4):77.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Universidad S, de Cali C, Soto AP, Daza Arana JE. Universidad Santiago de Cali, Gómez Ramírez E, Institución Universitaria Escuela Nacional del Deporte. Teorías generales que explican el movimiento corporal humano. In: Modelos teóricos para fisioterapia [Internet]. 1st ed. Editorial Universidad Santiago de Cali; 2020 [cited 2023 Dec 9]. pp. 35–52. https://libros.usc.edu.co/index.php/usc/catalog/view/145/185/2624-1.

  14. Correia V, Pereira E, Carvalho J, Minhalma RJP. Estudos em desenvolvimento motor da criança XV [Internet]. Universidade do Algarve - Escola Superior de Educação e Comunicação; 2022 [cited 2023 Dec 19]. https://sapientia.ualg.pt/handle/10400.1/18478.

  15. Ortega FB, Ruiz JR, Castillo MJ, Sjöström M. Physical fitness in childhood and adolescence: a powerful marker of health. Int J Obes. 2008;32(1):1–11.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Blair C. Educating executive function. WIREs Cogn Sci. 2017;8(1–2):e1403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64(1):135–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. McCoy DC. Measuring Young Children’s executive function and self-regulation in classrooms and other real-world settings. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2019;22(1):63–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Donnelly JE, Hillman CH, Castelli D, Etnier JL, Lee S, Tomporowski P, et al. Physical activity, fitness, cognitive function, and academic achievement in children: a systematic review. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2016;48(6):1197–222.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Sherry E. Stephen F. Wilson. Manual Oxford de medicina deportiva. Primera edición. Paidotribo; 772 p.

  21. Cadenas-Sanchez C, Intemann T, Labayen I, Peinado AB, Vidal-Conti J, Sanchis-Moysi J, et al. Physical fitness reference standards for preschool children: the PREFIT project. J Sci Med Sport. 2019;22(4):430–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Cadenas-Sanchez C, Martinez-Tellez B, Sanchez-Delgado G, Mora-Gonzalez J, Castro-Piñero J, Löf M, et al. Assessing physical fitness in preschool children: feasibility, reliability and practical recommendations for the PREFIT battery. J Sci Med Sport. 2016;19(11):910–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Trajković N, Rančić D, Ilić T, Herodek R, Korobeynikov G, Pekas D. Measuring handgrip strength in school children: inter-instrument reliability between Takei and Jamar. Sci Rep. 2024;14(1):1074.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Herrmann C, Gerlach E, Seelig H. Development and validation of a test instrument for the Assessment of Basic Motor competencies in Primary School. Meas Phys Educ Exerc Sci. 2015;19(2):80–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Berk LE. Development through the lifespan. 4. ed., Pearson International Edition. Boston, Mass. Munich: Pearson/Allyn and Bacon; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Hardy LL, Barnett L, Espinel P, Okely AD. Thirteen-year trends in child and adolescent Fundamental Movement skills: 1997–2010. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2013;45(10):1965–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Robinson LE, Rudisill ME, Goodway JD. Instructional climates in Preschool Children who are At-Risk. Part II: Perceived Physical competence. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2009;80(3):543–51.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Dobell A, Pringle A, Faghy MA, Roscoe CMP. Fundamental Movement skills and Accelerometer-measured physical activity levels during early childhood: a systematic review. Children. 2020;7(11):224.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Quitério A, Martins J, Onofre M, Costa J, Mota Rodrigues J, Gerlach E, et al. MOBAK 1 Assessment in Primary Physical Education: exploring Basic Motor competences of Portuguese 6-Year-Olds. Percept Mot Skills. 2018;125(6):1055–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Wälti M, Sallen J, Adamakis M, Ennigkeit F, Gerlach E, Heim C, et al. Basic Motor competencies of 6- to 8-Year-old primary School children in 10 European countries: a cross-sectional study on associations with Age, Sex, Body Mass Index, and physical activity. Front Psychol. 2022;13:804753.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Webster EK, Martin CK, Staiano AE. Fundamental motor skills, screen-time, and physical activity in preschoolers. J Sport Health Sci. 2019;8(2):114–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Behan S, Belton S, Peers C, O’Connor NE, Issartel J. Moving well-being well: investigating the maturation of fundamental movement skill proficiency across sex in Irish children aged five to twelve. J Sports Sci. 2019;37(22):2604–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. World Health Organization. Guidelines on physical activity, sedentary behaviour and sleep for children under 5 years of age [Internet]. Geneva: World Health Organization. 2019 [cited 2024 Jan 17]. 33 p. https://iris.who.int/handle/10665/311664.

  34. Roebers CM, Kauer M. Motor and cognitive control in a normative sample of 7-year‐olds. Dev Sci. 2009;12(1):175–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Scott J, Jay T, Spray C. Achievement in Fundamental Movement skills, spatial abilities, and Mathematics among Lower Key Stage 2 children. J Intell. 2023;11(5):87.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Cadenas-Sanchez C, Intemann T, Labayen I, Peinado AB, Vidal-Conti J, Sanchis-Moysi J, et al. Physical fitness reference standards for preschool children: The PREFIT project. J Sci Med Sport. 2019 Apr;22(4):430?7.

  37. Tomkinson GR, Lang JJ, Tremblay MS. Temporal trends in the cardiorespiratory fitness of children and adolescents representing 19 high-income and upper middle-income countries between 1981 and 2014. Br J Sports Med. 2019;53(8):478–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Graham M, Azevedo L, Wright M, Innerd AL. The effectiveness of Fundamental Movement Skill interventions on moderate to vigorous physical activity levels in 5- to 11-Year-old children: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Sports Med. 2022;52(5):1067–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Engel AC, Broderick CR, Van Doorn N, Hardy LL, Parmenter BJ. Exploring the relationship between Fundamental Motor Skill interventions and physical activity levels in children: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Sports Med. 2018;48(8):1845–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Lopes L, Santos R, Coelho-e-Silva M, Draper C, Mota J, Jidovtseff B, et al. A narrative review of motor competence in children and adolescents: what we know and what we need to find out. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;18(1):18.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Jones D, Innerd A, Giles EL, Azevedo LB. Association between fundamental motor skills and physical activity in the early years: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Sport Health Sci. 2020;9(6):542–52.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Metcalfe J, Clark J. The mountain of motor development: A metaphor. In: Motor Development: Research and Reviews. 2002. pp. 163–90.

  43. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. New York, NY, USA: Routledge Academic; 1988

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to acknowledge the parents and/or legal guardians who supported the study and participants of this study.

Funding

Funding for the study was provided by the Gobierno de Navarra–Departamento de Salud, co-financed by the European Regional Development Fund through the ERDF Operational Program 2014–2020 (Navarra).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

GLG, YGA, RRV, AMA, MI designed research; GLG, YGA, RRV, LAM, AMA conducted research; GLG, YGA, RRV, AMA analyzed data; GLG, YGA, RRV, MI AMA wrote the first draft of manuscript; GLG, AMA, RRV and MI had primary responsibility for final content. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mikel Izquierdo.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

We obtained informed consent from parents or guardians, and children provided informed assent before participating. The protocol aligned with the Declaration of Helsinki following Ethics Committee of the Department of Health of Navarra approval (PI_2021/111).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Conflict of interest

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Legarra-Gorgoñon, G., García-Alonso, Y., Ramírez-Vélez, R. et al. Assessing basic motor competences, physical fitness, and executive function in 4-5-year-old children: a longitudinal study in a primary care setting. Ital J Pediatr 50, 108 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13052-024-01674-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13052-024-01674-1

Keywords