Skip to main content

ADHD prevalence estimates in Italian children and adolescents: a methodological issue

Abstract

Background

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is recognized as the most common, and most studied, developmental age disorder. Basic information, such as the most appropriate case definition and the best way to evaluate the disorder’s prevalence rate, however, remains an open issue.

Methods

A comprehensive meta-analysis on the epidemiology of ADHD in Italy, which was lacking from the literature, was therefore performed to attempt to estimate the actual prevalence rate of ADHD, highlighting conceptual and quantitative differences between clinical-diagnosis and survey-based symptoms studies. The Medline, Embase, and PsycINFO databases, and the grey literature, were searched up to January 2018. The review was laid out in three main sections: an overall prevalence estimate, an epidemiological profile of ADHD symptoms, and an attempt to define the actual rate of ADHD diagnosis, as emerged from Italian studies.

Results

A total of 15 unique studies were included. These contributed to estimating the prevalence of ADHD in 67,838 subjects aged 5–17, representing 9 of the 20 regions (45%) of Italy. Overall, the pooled prevalence of ADHD was 2.9% (range: 1.1–16.7%). When distinguishing studies based on case definition, however, we found an average prevalence estimate, based on symptoms criteria, of 5.9% (range: 1.4 to 16.7%) and a best-estimate prevalence rate of 1.4% (range: 1.1 to 3.1%).

Conclusions

Following the case definition for epidemiological studies of ADHD, counting only subjects with an ADHD diagnosis performed and confirmed by clinical assessment would reduce the wide variability in prevalence estimates, and, above all, would both describe the real rate of subjects suffering from ADHD disorder and avoid misdiagnosis.

Background

The exact time of onset for individual cases in psychiatry is often not known, and prevalence estimates on a given period are used as a substitute for identifying the proportion of cases of a particular disorder in a defined population [1].

Several factors can influence observed prevalence rates as the diagnostic criteria, the setting, the population studied, the type and severity of the disorder, and the comorbidities. Since measures of prevalence are also helpful in assessing health care needs and in planning health care services [2], estimates should be as accurate as possible. This is an issue especially in mental health care, where the risk of misdiagnosis and of false positives is a significant problem affecting appropriate and effective interventions, increasing the risk of medicalization and overuse of drug treatments, and creating stigma and discrimination [3, 4], as the worldwide debate also on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) confirms [5].

To date, ADHD is considered the most common, and most studied, developmental age disorder, even though basic information, such as the most appropriate case definition for estimating its prevalence rate, remains an open issue and leaves room for significant, debates in scientific literature [6, 7]. The reported range in prevalence is very wide (from 0.2 to 34.5%), and heterogeneity in the methodological approaches used contributes to these differences [8,9,10,11,12]. This is similar to the situation described for all child and mental health problems worldwide [13], supporting difficulties, greater in psychiatry than physical medicine, in discriminating disordered and non-disordered conditions [14, 15]. The 5% increase in children diagnosed as having ADHD reported in the US in recent years also suggests the need for valid tools to support diagnosis in practice [16].

ADHD is recognized as a difficult diagnosis to make accurately, not only because of the many comorbid conditions, but also for the low specificity of the core symptoms: the list of disorders or conditions that can make a child appear restless or distractible is almost endless. Making a proper diagnosis thus requires a detailed evaluation of development, educational demands, and what is expected of the child in a given circumstance and at a given time, as well as symptomatology, impairment, and risk [17, 18].

Although it is widely recognized that several ADHD symptoms, as investigated by symptom surveys or interviews, may occur as manifestations of other medical (i.e. hypoglycemia or sensory processing disorders) and psychiatric (i.e. mood, anxiety or autism spectrum disorders) disorders, few published studies have directly examined the rate and type of psychiatric and medical disorders in those previously identified as ADHD positive by teacher or parent ratings [19]. All psychiatric diagnoses are mainly clinical-based, and both subjectivity and cultural factors affect the evaluation of symptom severity (significant distress) and impairment (in social, academic or occupational functioning) in the disorder [20, 21]. Furthermore, subjects defined as having ADHD according to symptom survey-based evaluations may not truly be suffering from ADHD because they may meet only one of the five DSM-IV-TR criteria (criteria A) needed to reach a diagnosis of ADHD, and a clinical evaluation is necessary to assess the other four criteria [22]. The risk of “misdiagnosing normality” in psychiatry is high, in particular when symptom-based criteria for disorders, as in the DSM, are applied using symptom checklists, in particular in population settings and by non-medical-health professionals [14].

In such a context the goal of this study was to evaluate certain factors that can affect the prevalence of ADHD in reported studies and, consequently, the observed variability, in particular in overall pooled estimates. We analyzed the methodological approach used, and the implications in practice, of Italian ADHD studies.

Methods

The research was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Istituto di Ricerche Farmacologiche Mario Negri IRCCS.

Search strategy and selection criteria

We searched the Medline, Embase, and PsycINFO databases for articles written in English and published before January 2018 using the following Medical Subject Headings and free text terms: “ADHD”, “ADD”, “attention deficit”, “attention deficit hyperactivity disorder”, “hyperkinetic disorder”, “epidemiology”, “prevalence”, “survey”, “child*”, and “adolesc*”. Studies with an Italian affiliation and point prevalence estimates of ADHD in Italy were extracted. Non indexed journals were searched for in the Google Scholar search engine by using keywords to identify potentially eligible studies. Articles written in Italian were also considered. Additionally, the reference lists of all eligible articles were scanned, as well as key Italian journals and websites, to identify additional, potentially relevant papers. Studies considered eligible were those that used the diagnostic criteria or survey instruments based on DSM-III, DSM-III-R, DSM-IV, DSM-5, or ICD-10, with samples from community, school, or clinically referred populations. We included studies with participants aged < 18 years.

Data extraction and quality assessment

After removing duplicates, the two authors screened the titles and abstracts for adherence to eligibility criteria. In cases of uncertainty concerning eligibility, the records were discussed until a consensus was achieved. For studies deemed suitable, we obtained the full text for data extraction. References of suitable studies were searched to recover any relevant articles.

Data were extracted by the authors and involved general publication information, demographic variables of the population sample, year of sampling, setting, frame procedure, region and city, screening and diagnostic instruments used to define a case as ADHD, informant, and whether a clinical impairment evaluation was performed. The authors independently assessed each Italian study for methodological quality. The included articles were assessed by using a modified tool developed by Hoy et al. [23] for assessing risk of bias in prevalence studies that includes eight questions. These were: 1) was the study’s target population a close representation of the national population in relation to relevant variables?; 2) was the sampling frame a true or close representation of the target population?; 3) was some form of random selection used to select the sample?; 4) was the likelihood of non response bias minimal?; 5) were data collected directly from the subjects?; 6) was the study instrument that measured the parameter of interest shown to have validity and reliability?; 7) was the same mode of data collection used for all subjects?; 8) were the numerator(s) and denominator(s) for the parameter of interest appropriate? A study was considered to have a high overall risk of bias if ≤3 criteria were met, moderate risk of bias if 4 or 5 criteria were met, and low risk of bias if 6 to 8 criteria were met.

Statistical analysis

For each retrieved study an assessment of inclusion, exclusion, and quality was performed independently by the two authors, and the inter-reviewer reliability was measured using Cohen’s Kappa statistics. Study data were analysed using Stata version 11.1 (Stata Corp, College Station, TX). Because of the differences in study sample sizes, SEs of the prevalence estimates from each study were calculated based on the exact binomial likelihood. Summary effect estimates of prevalence were calculated by using a random effects meta-regression model because there was a clear heterogeneity between the studies tested using the I2 statistic [24]. The study factors that might be related to prevalence estimate were first tested individually in a univariate analysis and then simultaneously in a multiple meta-regression model via likelihood ratio test conducted with R, using the ‘metafor’ package [25]. Study factors included: sample size, sampling frame, informant, quality of the study, geographical location, and diagnostic criteria. Studies were grouped according to considered factors, and the estimates were then pooled. We used the z test of 2 proportions to examine differences in prevalence estimates of studies by factors considered. Five studies reported prevalence estimates from different types of informants. These studies were included in univariate and multiple meta-regression analyses and in the overall pooled results for each prevalence estimate.

Results

Our search yielded 199 citations, 5 of which were duplicates (Fig. 1). After removal of unsuitable and ineligible studies, and the addition of 9 papers retrieved from the bibliographies of identified studies or grey literature, we had a total of 15 unique Italian studies for quality assessment and meta-analysis [26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40]. Included studies contributed 22 estimates of prevalence in 67,838 subjects, 5–17 years old, over a 30-year period (Table 1). Male sample was reported in 12 studies and lay within the range 45–55%. Both reviewers fully agreed on the choice of the pertinent studies (weighted K = 1). Studies meeting inclusion criteria were conducted in 9 of 20 Italian regions (45%) that cover 53% of the Italian 5–17 year old population. However, 7 studies (47%) were conducted in the North of the country. Among the 9 studies (60%) reporting the year of sampling the time period between the data collection and the publication ranged from 1 to 11 years (average 4). A majority of the studies was conducted in school populations (n = 12), while the rest were performed in clinical settings (2 in child and adolescent neuropsychiatric services and 1 in family paediatrician practices), using a whole population approach. Three of the school-based studies also involved child and adolescent neuropsychiatric services in the clinical confirmation of suspect ADHD. Overall, the methods used in the studies were rating scales, questionnaires, interviews, or other clinical tools based mainly on DSM-IV criteria (11 studies), while the remaining were conducted according to DSM-III-R (2 studies), DSM-III (1 study) and ICD-10 (1 study) criteria. The informants in the studies were teachers in 11 cases, patients in 1, and parents in 3; 2 studies included both teachers and parents.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow diagram of Italian study retrieval and selectiongmx

Table 1 Characteristics of Italian Studies evaluated for prevalence of ADHD

A good agreement between reviewers on the evaluation of the quality of the studies was found (weighted K = 0.61). No studies met all 8 criteria, although 93% had a low, or moderate, risk of bias. The majority of studies rated poorly for the representativeness of sample (87%).

The overall, pooled prevalence of ADHD, including all reported prevalence estimates (n = 22), was 4.3% (95% confidence interval [CI]:3.1 to 5.7), with a wide inter-study range of 1.1 to 16.7% (Table 1). The prevalence estimate of ADHD was, on average, 0.5% lower including, in the overall pooled analysis, only the lowest estimate of each study (3.8%, CI 2.6–5.1). In only one study of low quality the prevalence estimate of ADHD was lower for males than female [38], while in all other studies the rate for boys was 1.2–7.6 higher than for girls.

The included studies used different algorithms to estimate the number of children and adolescents with ADHD. To examine the impact of these different assessment procedures, separate prevalence estimates were calculated for each specific algorithm used. The majority of studies defined ADHD based on symptom ratings by teachers alone (8 studies). Only 2 studies required an individual to meet symptom criteria based on both parent and teacher ratings, using the “AND rule” algorithm that codes as positive only if both rates agree. Finally, 6 of 15 studies (40%) used a best estimate diagnostic algorithm in which a clinical evaluation was performed at the end of the assessment to obtain an ADHD diagnosis based on standard classification criteria (4 based on DSM-IV, 1 on DSM-III-R, 1 on ICD-10, and no study on DSM-5). Four of these 6 studies assessed a population sample and 2 a population of clinically referred subjects.

Within the univariate models, prevalence estimates for ADHD were, on average, 1.7% lower when DSM-IV criteria were used than when other criteria were used (Table 2). One study was conducted in the North, Centre, and South of Italy and prevalence estimates for ADHD were, on average, 2.2% lower compared to the North, and close to those of other geographical locations. On average, similar ADHD prevalence estimates were obtained when the informant was the clinician or both the parent and teacher (AND rule), whereas estimates were 5.1% higher when based on teacher ratings, 1% higher when based on parental reports, and 15% higher when based on child interviews. There was a significant increase in prevalence estimates when the school setting was compared with that of the population (at school estimates were, on average 3.9% higher). Prevalence estimates were, on average, 2.8% lower when study sample sizes were > 1000 participants, and 1.5% higher when the quality of the study was low or moderate (RoB score ≤ 5). Prevalence estimates were, on average, slightly lower in studies published before 2006.

Table 2 Association between study factors and ADHD estimates

According to both the clinical and methodological diversity of the retrieved studies, all univariate analyses revealed that all considered covariates were significantly associated with heterogeneity of prevalence estimates.

After entering all study factors into a multivariate meta-regression, only teacher and child case definition remained significant (Table 2).

Discussion

Performing a clinical assessment to make a diagnosis in mental health care is necessary to define whether a subject suffers from a psychiatric disorder or not. In our opinion, therefore, selecting an appropriate case definition that is supported by a clinician-based diagnosis is a critical step to estimating the real prevalence rate of ADHD.

This opinion is not entirely new: 20 years ago, Swanson et al. [6] clearly wrote that an ADHD diagnosis should be based on clinical history since this allows one to define “the combination of inattentive, hyperactive, and impulsive behaviour as a disorder when these behaviours are severe, developmentally inappropriate, and impair function at home and school”. They continued on to say that “rating scales, with the specific ADHD symptoms, have been developed and provide a systematic approach for documenting clinical history, but these are commonly compromised by rater-specific effects and thus should be confirmed by interview”. This may be the main factor explaining the wide variation in prevalence estimates reported by the numerous individual studies and meta-analyses. Although documented, this study limitation is often ignored and thus increases the controversy over whether ADHD is overdiagnosed or underdiagnosed and the true prevalence rate of the disorder. The appraisal we performed of the meta-analyses confirms the weakness of the reported overall ADHD prevalence rates. Once again, also in our sample, the observed overlap of studies analysed in the meta-analyses seems unnecessary and may reflect a waste of efforts and an inefficiency in the process of summarizing evidence [41].

We performed a systematic evaluation of the rate of ADHD children and adolescents in Italy with the overall aim to distinguish studies estimating ADHD prevalence based only on symptom-surveys from studies providing a clinically comprehensive evaluation. We also aimed to provide an overall ADHD estimate, as done in previous reviews. The first rate we computed was the overall Italian prevalence of ADHD, found considering all types of samples with different case definition methodologies. As expected, considering Italy’s ADHD history – characterized by a predominantly psychodynamic-psychoanalytic approach [42] – the overall prevalence rate of 2.9%, ranging from 1.1 to 16.7% is lower than the worldwide estimate of 5.29% [43]. Our rate and/or range are similar to those that emerged from older review studies [44,45,46,47], while they differ more from those of the most recent studies [8, 9, 11, 12, 43, 48, 49]. These data lead to significant concern that there is inconsistent, wide variability, not only between the rates found in the original studies, but also between the findings of the reviews, suggesting the need to consider which frame, diagnosis criteria, and instruments the estimated and reported ADHD prevalence rates refer. According to other authors [6, 50], the prevalence of ADHD symptoms and the prevalence of ADHD diagnosis are rates that should be carefully differentiated from each other because they reflect two different subject populations, and they are also populations with different health care needs.

The first population (ADHD symptom rate) is the number of subjects presenting ADHD symptoms who could have an ADHD disorder or another psychiatric or medical disorder with similar clinical manifestations. This population is often recognized through symptom surveys compiled by parents or teachers, and needs a clinical evaluation to confirm whether ADHD is actually present. This population therefore represents the number of children and adolescents with behavioral symptoms of ADHD who need a specific evaluation by a specialist service/clinician.

From our findings, this population in Italy, calculated from studies with data rates based on only symptom-surveys, consists of about 439,000 subjects (5.9%, range: 1.4–16.7) among children and adolescents aged 5 to 17 years of the Italian paediatric population. This is, from a health care point of view, the population of subjects who need a psychiatric evaluation. This rate, based on symptom-surveys, differs from that found with the same methodological approach in Thomas and colleagues’ review (5.9 vs. 13%) [12], as well as from those based on parent (2.5%, range: 2.4–2.5%) and teacher ratings (6.7%, range: 4.5–10.8%), which are both lower compared to similar, previous analyses on the literature [8, 9, 47, 49]. This result could also be expected, however, taking into consideration that cultural factors, such as higher symptom tolerance, may modulate the interpretation of the child’s behaviours in parent and teacher evaluations [42, 51].

The second rate, the prevalence of ADHD diagnosis, is, according to us, the real rate of ADHD prevalence and refers to the number of patients presenting ADHD symptoms who have an ADHD diagnosis confirmed by a clinical evaluation. This population, similarly to the previous one, can be recognized through symptom surveys compiled by parents or teachers, but has an ADHD diagnosis and evaluation that confirm the presence of ADHD. This second population represents the number of children and adolescents with an ADHD diagnosis who need a specific treatment for ADHD.

From our findings, in Italy this population (ADHD diagnosis), calculated from studies including only patients with an ADHD diagnosis confirmed by clinical evaluation, consists of about 105,000 subjects (1.4%, range: 1.1–3.1) among the Italian paediatric population aged 5 to 17 years. From a health care point of view, this is the population of patients who need treatment. In line with previous comments, only a few review studies calculated the prevalence of ADHD diagnosis separately from the overall rate. It is even more important to keep in mind the distinction between diagnostic procedures if we consider that our findings (1.4%, range: 1.1–3.1) differ from those of Willcutt [9] (5.9%, range: 4.6–7.5) even when the better estimate diagnostic procedure is employed.

Finally, although we found similar rates between the overall ADHD prevalence (2.9%; range: 1.1–16.7) and the ADHD diagnosis prevalence (1.4%, range: 1.1–3.1), when diagnostic case definition based on clinical evaluation is used, comparisons of the range rates suggest that the differences between these homogenous types of studies are small, and, thus, in our opinion, more accurate.

Results should be interpreted in the context of two main limitations. First, the number of studies included is small and the methodological approach is heterogeneous so the findings that emerged may not be similar to those of other contexts or countries. Second, although this is the first study analyzing the overall ADHD prevalence in Italy, all data originate from a single country. This may affect the comparability of the reported findings and the generalization of the results to a worldwide scenario would therefore be inappropriate.

Conclusions

Epidemiological studies concerning ADHD need more efforts to identify the cases, to assess the prevalence, and to use administrative databases as provided by the American Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ) [52]. In our opinion, considering only subjects with an ADHD diagnosis performed and confirmed by full clinical assessment (according to European and international guidelines) as a case definition for epidemiological ADHD studies would reduce the wide variability in ADHD estimates previously described. Above all, it would represent the real rate of subjects suffering from ADHD disorder and would avoid misdiagnosis.

Mental health is certainly a public health issue, and many disorders arise in childhood. To support the promotion of mental wellbeing and the primary prevention of psychiatric condition [53] knowledge of the true dimension of the problem – in this case the ADHD prevalence – is fundamental for planning and achieving appropriate treatments and interventions.

Abbreviations

ADHD:

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

AHRQ:

American Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality

AR:

“and rule” algorithm

CANPS:

Child and Adolescent Neuropsychiatric Service

CI:

Confidence interval

CTRS:

Conners’ Teacher Rating Scale

DBD:

Disruptive Behavior Disorder rating scale

DICA-R:

Diagnostic Interview for Child and Adolescent

DSM:

Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders

DSM-III:

Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Third edition

DSM-III-R:

Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Third edition. Revised

DSM-IV:

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental disorders. Fourth edition.

DSM-IV-TR:

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental disorders. Fourth edition. Text revision

FP:

Family Pediatrician

ICD-10:

International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems 10th Revision

NR:

Not reported

RoB score:

Risk of Bias Assessment tool

SDAG:

Attention and Hyperactivity Parent rating scale

SDAI:

Attention and Hyperactivity Teacher rating scale

SEs:

Standard error

SQ:

Study questionnaire

VADTRS:

Vanderbilt Diagnostic ADHD Teacher Rating Scale

References

  1. Kleinbaum DG, Kupper LL, Morgenstern H. Epidemiologic Research: Principles and Quantitiative Methods. Belmont CA: Lifetime Learning Publications; 1982.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Coghill DR. Organisation of services for managing ADHD. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci. 2017;26:453–8.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Wakefield JC. DSM-5, psychiatric epidemiology and the false positives problem. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci. 2015;24(3):188–96.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Thornicroft G, Mehta N, Clement S, Evans-Lacko S, Doherty M, Rose D, et al. Evidence for effective interventions to reduce mental-health-related stigma and discrimination. Lancet. 2016;387(10023):1123–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Singh I. Beyond polemics: science and ethics of ADHD. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:957–64.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Swanson JM, Sergeant JA, Taylor E, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Jensen PS, Cantwell DP. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and hyperkinetic disorder. Lancet. 1998;351(9100):429–33.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Thomas R, Mitchell GK, Batstra L. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: are we helping or harming? Br Med J. 2013;347:f6172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Polanczyk G, Jensen P. Epidemiologic considerations in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a review and update. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2008;17(2):245–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Willcutt EG. The prevalence of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Neurotherapeutics. 2012;9(3):490–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Polanczyk GV, Willcutt EG, Salum GA, Kieling C, Rohde LA. ADHD prevalence estimates across three decades: an updated systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(2):434–42.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Polanczyk GV, Salum GA, Sugaya LS, Caye A, Rohde LA. Annual research review: a meta-analysis of the worldwide prevalence of mental disorders in children and adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56(3):345–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Thomas R, Sanders S, Doust J, Beller E, Glasziou P. Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2015;135:e994–e1001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Kieling C, Baker-Henningham H, Belfer M, Conti G, Ertem I, Omigbodun O, et al. Child and adolescent mental health worldwide: evidence for action. Lancet. 2011;378:1515–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Warkefield JC. Misdiagnosis normality: psychiatry’s failure to address the problem of false positive diagnoses of mental disorder in a changing professional environment. J Ment Health. 2010;19:337–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Singh I, Wessely S. Childhood: a suitable case for treatment? Lancet Psychiatry. 2015;2(7):661–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Mc Cartky M. Most ADHD diagnoses made in US children seem to follow guidelines. Br Med J. 2015;351:h4946.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Rowland AS, Lesesne CA, Abramowitz AJ. The epidemiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a public health view. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2002;8(3):162–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Timimi S, Taylor E. ADHD is best understood as a cultural construct. Br J Psychiatry. 2004;184:8–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Narad ME, Garner AA, Peugh JL, Tamm L, Antonini TN, Kingery KM, et al. Parent-teacher agreement on ADHD symptoms across development. Psychol Assess. 2015;27:239–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Bonati M, Reale L. Reducing overdiagnosis and disease mongering in ADHD in Lombardy. Br Med J. 2013;16:347.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Jackson T. Attention, please. Br Med J. 1994;347:f6697.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Hoy D, Brooks P, Woolf A, Blyth F, March L, Bain C, et al. Assessing risk of bias in prevalence studies: modification of an existing tool and evidence of interrater agreement. J Clin Epidemiol. 2012;65(9):934–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Higgins JPT, Thompson SG. Controlling the risk of spurious findings from meta-regression. Stat Med. 2004;23:1663–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Viechtbauer W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J Stat Softw. 2010;36:1–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. O'Leary KD, Vivian D, Nisi A. Hyperactivity in Italy. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1985;13(4):485–500.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Gallucci F, Bird HR, Berardi C, Gallai V, Pfanner P, Weinberg A. Symptoms of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in an Italian school sample: findings of a pilot study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1993;32(5):1051–8.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Camerini GB, Coccia M, Caffo E. Il disturbo da deficit dell'attenzione iperattività: analisi della frequenza in una popolazione scolastica attraverso questionari agli insegnanti. Psichiatria Dell'Infanzia e Dell'Adolescenza. 1996;63:587–94.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Marzocchi GM, Cornoldi C. Una Scala di Facile uso per la Rilevazione dei Comportamenti Problematici dei Bambini con Deficit di Attenzione e Iperattività. Psicol Clin Svilupp. 2000;4:43–63.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Corbo S, Marolla F, Sarno V, Torrioli MG, Vernacotola S. Prevalenza dell'ADHD in bambini seguiti dal Pediatra di Famiglia. Medico e Bambino. 2003;1:22–5.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Ciotti F. La sindrome ipercinetica “pura” fra gli alunni nel territorio cesenate. Quaderni ACP. 2003;4:18–20.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Madeddu F, Dazzi S, Prunas A, Barzaghi A, Ripamonti C, Schadee H. Prevalenza del Disturbo da Deficit dell’Attenzione/Iperattività in un ampio campione di preadolescenti italiani. Minerva Psichiatr. 2006;47(3):209–20.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Mugnaini D, Masi G, Brovedani P, Chelazzi C, Matas M, Romagnoli C, et al. Teacher reports of ADHD symptoms in Italian children at the end of first grade. Eur Psychiatry. 2006;21(6):419–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Zuddas A, Marzocchi GM, Oosterlaan J, Cavolina P, Ancilletta B, Sergeant J. Factor structure and cultural aspects of disruptive behaviour disorders symptoms in Italian children. Eur Psychiatry. 2006;21:410–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Faravelli C, Lo Sauro C, Castellini G, Ricca V, Pallanti S. Prevalence and correlates of mental disorders in a school-survey sample. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health. 2009;5:1–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Maschietto D, Baioni E, Vio C, Novello F, Germinario EAP, Regini FM, et al. Prevalenza dell’adhd in una popolazione pediatrica e sua esposizione al trattamento psico–comportamentale e farmacologico. Medico e Bambino pagine elettroniche. 2012;15(10) https://www.medicoebambino.com/?id=RIC1210_10.html). Accessed 3 Aug 2018

  37. Bianchini R, Postorino V, Grasso R, Santoro B, Migliore S, Burlò C, Tata C, Mazzone L. Prevalence of ADHD in a sample of Italian students: a population-based study. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34(9):2543–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Gritti A, Bravaccio C, Signoriello S, Salerno F, Pisano S, Catone G, et al. Epidemiological study on behavioural and emotional problems in developmental age: prevalence in a sample of Italian children, based on parent and teacher reports. Ital J Pediatr. 2014;40:19.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Donfrancesco R, Marano A, Calderoni D, Mugnaini D, Thomas F, Di Trani M, et al. Prevalence of severe ADHD: an epidemiological study in the Italian regions of Tuscany and Latium. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci. 2015;24(6):525–33.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Zucchetti G, Ortega E, Scholte RHJ, Rabaglietti E. The mediating role of aggressive behaviour, emotional and Behavioural instability on the association between ADHD symptoms and best friend conflicts. Curr Psychol. 2015;34:97–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Siontis KC, Hernandez-Boussard T, Ioannidis JP. Overlapping meta-analyses on the same topic: survey of published studies. Br Med J. 2013;347:f4501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Conrad P, Bergey MR. The impending globalization of ADHD: notes on the expansion and growth of a medicalized disorder. Soc Sci Med. 2014;122:31–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Polanczyk G, de Lima MS, Horta BL, Biederman J, Rohde LA. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:942–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Goldman LS, Genel M, Bezman RJ, Slanetz PJ. Diagnosis and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Council on Scientific Affairs, American Medical Association. JAMA. 1998;279(14):1100–7.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  45. Elia J, Ambrosini PJ, Rapoport JL. Treatment of attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. N Engl J Med. 1999;340(10):780–8.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Brown RT, Freeman WS, Perrin JM, Stein MT, Amler RW, Feldman HM, et al. Prevalence and assessment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in primary care settings. Pediatrics. 2001;107(3):E43.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Faraone SV, Sergeant J, Gillberg C, Biederman J. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: is it an American condition? World Psychiatry. 2003;2(2):104–13.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Erskine HE, Ferrari AJ, Polanczyk GV, Moffitt TE, Murray CJ, Vos T, et al. The global burden of conduct disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in 2010. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55(4):328–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Skounti M, Philalithis A, Galanakis E. Variations in prevalence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder worldwide. Eur J Pediatr. 2007;166(2):117–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. McKeown RE, Holbrook JR, Danielson ML, Cuffe SP, Wolraich ML, Visser SN. The impact of case definition on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder prevalence estimates in community-based samples of school-aged children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015;54(1):53–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Rohde LA, Szobot C, Polanczyk G, Schmitz M, Martins S, Tramontina S. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a diverse culture: do research and clinical findings support the notion of a cultural construct for the disorder? Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(11):1436–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Gaynes BN, Christian R, Saavedra LM, Wines R, Jonas DE, Viswanathan M, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: identifying high priority future research needs. J Psychiatr Pract. 2014;20(2):104–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Mental Health Foundation. Better Mental Health for All: A Public Health Approach to Mental Health Improvement. London: Faculty of Public Health and Mental Health Foundation. https://www.mentalhealth.org.uk/publications/better-mental-health-all-public-health-approach-mental-health-improvement; 2016. Accessed 3 Aug 2018.

Download references

Acknowledgements

At the Istituto di Ricerche Farmacologiche Mario Negri IRCCS, Milan, Italy, Massimo Cartabia provided statistical analysis support and Chiara Pandolfini for language editing.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MB designed the study. Both authors contributed to the literature search, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, and manuscript writing.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Maurizio Bonati.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no conflicts or competing of interest.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Reale, L., Bonati, M. ADHD prevalence estimates in Italian children and adolescents: a methodological issue. Ital J Pediatr 44, 108 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13052-018-0545-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13052-018-0545-2

Keywords